Welcome to Open Science
Contact Us
Home Books Journals Submission Open Science Join Us News
Reward Sensitivity Relates to Automatic Approach Motivation for Exercise: Preliminary Validity for the Exercise-Approach Avoidance Task
Current Issue
Volume 6, 2019
Issue 2 (March)
Pages: 24-28   |   Vol. 6, No. 2, March 2019   |   Follow on         
Paper in PDF Downloads: 52   Since Jun. 27, 2019 Views: 1048   Since Jun. 27, 2019
Authors
[1]
Christine Nicole May, Department of Psychology, Springfield College, Springfield, USA.
[2]
James Juergensen, Department of Psychology, Youngstown State University, Youngstown, USA.
[3]
Sam Santich, Department of Exercise Science, Springfield College, Springfield, USA.
Abstract
Only about half (49.9%) of US college students met the physical activity recommendations set forth by the US Department of Health and Human Services. Most assessment and intervention methods use explicit behavior (e.g., self-reported physical activity) and neglect the salient role of implicit motives. There is a need for novel implicit methods for increasing physical activity. Previous research has used the Approach Avoidance Task (AAT) to measure implicit approach motivation for various objects, including desserts, alcohol, and spiders. People are quicker to approach to appetitive stimuli (e.g., desserts) and avoid fearful stimuli (e.g., spiders). We explored the validity of an adapted version of the AAT, the Exercise-AAT, by assessing the relationship between responsiveness to rewards/punishments (BIS/BAS) and responses on the Exercise-AAT. Forty-nine undergraduate students from a public, Midwestern university participated in this study. We conducted Pearson correlations to examine the relationship between BIS/BAS and relative approach motivation for exercise images (AppMot). BAS Reward Responsiveness and BAS Drive were not significantly related to AppMot (ps >.23). However, BAS Fun-Seeking was significantly and positively correlated with AppMot, r (47) = .28, p = .026. These results support previous research using the Dessert-AAT (D-AAT), which showed that BAS Fun-Seeking was significantly and positively related to implicit approach motivation for dessert images. The significant, positive relationship between BAS Fun-Seeking and relative approach motivation for exercise images in this study provides preliminary evidence for the validity of the Exercise-AAT. We discuss future treatment implications for individuals whose health would benefit from an increase in exercise.
Keywords
Approach, Avoidance, BAS, Reward, Exercise, Motivation, AAT
Reference
[1]
Piercy, K. L., Troiano, R. P., Ballard, R. M., Carlson, S. A., Fulton, J. E., Galuska, D. A., … Olson, R. D. (2018). The physical activity guidelines for Americans. JAMA - Journal of the American Medical Association. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2018.14854.
[2]
Dinger, M. K., Brittain, D. R., & Hutchinson, S. R. (2014). Associations between physical activity and health-related factors in a national sample of college students. Journal of American College Health. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2013.849710.
[3]
Calitri, R., Lowe, R., Eves, F. F., & Bennett, P. (2009). Associations between visual attention, implicit and explicit attitude and behaviour for physical activity. Psychology & Health, 24 (9), 1105–1123. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440802245306.
[4]
Conroy, D. E., Hyde, A. L., Doerksen, S. E., & Ribeiro, N. F. (2010). Implicit Attitudes and Explicit Motivation Prospectively Predict Physical Activity. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 39 (2), 112–118. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-010-9161-0.
[5]
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary Determinants of Attitudes. II: Arm Flexion and Extension Have Differential Effects on Attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.5.
[6]
Rinck, M., & Becker, E. S. (2007). Approach and avoidance in fear of spiders. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 38 (2), 105–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.001.
[7]
May, C. N., Nock, N. L., Bentley, D., & Demaree, H. A. (2016). Acute aerobic exercise increases implicit approach motivation for dessert images. Journal of Health Psychology, 135910531665740. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105316657404.
[8]
Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment : The BIS / BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67 (2), 319–333. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319.
[9]
Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain Systems that Mediate both Emotion and Cognition. Cognition and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939008410799.
[10]
Helfer, S. G., Elhai, J. D., & Geers, A. L. (2015). Affect and Exercise: Positive Affective Expectations Can Increase Post-Exercise Mood and Exercise Intentions. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9656-1.
[11]
Schneider, M., & Graham, D. (2009). Personality, physical fitness, and affective response to exercise among adolescents. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e31818de009.
[12]
Barrós-Loscertales, A., Ventura-Campos, N., Sanjuán-Tomás, A., Belloch, V., Parcet, M. A., & Ávila, C. (2010). Behavioral activation system modulation on brain activation during appetitive and aversive stimulus processing. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq012.
[13]
May, C. N., Juergensen, J., & Demaree, H. A. (2016). Yum, cake!: How reward sensitivity relates to automatic approach motivation for dessert food images. Personality and Individual Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.11.022.
[14]
Juergensen, J., & Leckfor, C. (2018). Stop Pushing Me Away: Relative Level of Facebook Addiction Is Associated With Implicit Approach Motivation for Facebook Stimuli. Psychological Reports. https://doi.org/10.1177/0033294118798624.
[15]
Marchewka, A., Zurawski, Ł., Jednoróg, K., & Grabowska, A. (2014). The Nencki Affective Picture System (NAPS): Introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high-quality, realistic picture database. Behavior Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0379-1.
[16]
Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22 (4), 490–497. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611400615.
[17]
Heym, N., Ferguson, E., & Lawrence, C. (2008). An evaluation of the relationship between Gray’s revised RST and Eysenck’s PEN: Distinguishing BIS and FFFS in Carver and White’s BIS/BAS scales. Personality and Individual Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.013.
[18]
Smillie, L. D., Jackson, C. J., & Dalgleish, L. I. (2006). Conceptual distinctions among Carver and White’s (1994) BAS scales: A reward-reactivity versus trait impulsivity perspective. Personality and Individual Differences. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.10.012.
[19]
Cheval, B., Radel, R., Neva, J. L., Boyd, L. A., Swinnen, S. P., Sander, D., & Boisgontier, M. P. (2018). Behavioral and Neural Evidence of the Rewarding Value of Exercise Behaviors: A Systematic Review. Sports Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-018-0898-0.
Open Science Scholarly Journals
Open Science is a peer-reviewed platform, the journals of which cover a wide range of academic disciplines and serve the world's research and scholarly communities. Upon acceptance, Open Science Journals will be immediately and permanently free for everyone to read and download.
CONTACT US
Office Address:
228 Park Ave., S#45956, New York, NY 10003
Phone: +(001)(347)535 0661
E-mail:
LET'S GET IN TOUCH
Name
E-mail
Subject
Message
SEND MASSAGE
Copyright © 2013-, Open Science Publishers - All Rights Reserved